Women were less accurate at understanding cardiac signals – such as counting their heartbeat. Women were less accurate at understanding cardiac signals – such as counting their heartbeat.

Differences in how men and women perceive internal body signals could have implications for mental health

Some call interoception our ‘sixth sense’.

Most people are familiar with the five senses (touch, sight, hearing, smell and taste), but not everyone knows that we have an additional sense called interoception. This is the sense of our body’s internal state. It helps us feel and interpret internal signals that regulate vital functions in our body, like hunger, thirst, body temperature and heart rate.

Although we don’t take much notice of it, it’s an extremely important sense as it ensures that every system in the body is working optimally. It does this by alerting us to when our body may be out of balance – such as making us reach for a drink when we feel thirsty or telling us to take our jumper off when we’re feeling too hot.

Interoception is also important for our mental health. This is because it contributes to many psychological processes - including decision making, social ability and emotional wellbeing.

Disrupted interoception is even reported in many mental health conditions – including depression, anxiety and eating disorders. It may also explain why many mental health conditions share similar symptoms – such as disturbed sleep or fatigue.

Despite how important interoception is to all aspects of our health, little is known about whether men and women differ in how accurately they sense their body’s internal signals. So far, studies that have investigated whether cisgender men and women (a person whose gender identity aligns with their biological sex) sense and interpret interoceptive signals from their heart, lungs and stomach differently have found mixed results. Finding out if differences exist is important, as it may improve our understanding of differences in mental and physical health.

To get a clearer picture, we combined data from 93 studies looking at interoception in men and women. We focused on studies that looked at how people perceive heart, lung and stomach signals across a range of different tasks. For example, some studies had participants count their heart beats, while others asked participants to determine whether a flashing light happened when their stomach contracted, or tested if they could detect a difference in their breath while breathing into a device that makes it more difficult to do so normally.

Our analysis found that interoception does in fact differ between men and women. Women were significantly less accurate at heart-focused tasks (and to some extent lung-focused tasks) compared with men. These differences do not seem to be explained by other factors – such as how hard participants tried during the task, or physiological differences, such as body weight or blood pressure.

Women were less accurate at understanding cardiac signals – such as counting their heartbeat.Women were less accurate at understanding cardiac signals – such as counting their heartbeat.

Though we found significant differences across heartbeat tasks, results for other tasks were less clear. This might be because only a small proportion of studies have looked at lung and stomach perception. It might be too early to tell whether men and women differ in their perception of these signals.

Mental health

Our findings may be important for helping us understand why many common mental health conditions (such as anxiety and depression) are more prevalent in women than men from puberty onward. Several theories have been proposed to explain this – such as genetics, hormones, personality and exposure to stress or childhood adversity.

But because we know that interoception is important for wellbeing, it could be possible that differences in interoception may partly explain why more women suffer from anxiety and depression than men. This is because difficulties with interoception can affect many areas, including emotional, social and cognitive function, which are all known risk factors for many mental health conditions.

Knowing the differences in how men and women sense interoceptive signals may also be important for treating mental illness. While new studies suggest improving interoception improves mental health, studies also suggest that men may use interoceptive signals – for example from their heart – more than women when processing their emotions.

Other differences have also been reported, with studies suggesting that women pay more attention to interoceptive signals than men. This could mean that treatments that target or seek to improve interoception may work better for some people, or that different techniques may work better for others. This is something future research will need to investigate.

But while we know these differences exist, we still don’t know what causes them. Researchers have a few theories, including the distinct physiological and hormonal changes most men and women experience. It may also be caused by differences in how many men and women are taught to think about their emotions or interoceptive signals, like pain. Better understanding all the factors that affect interoceptive ability may be important for someday developing better treatments for many mental health conditions.

REGISTER NOW

By Jennifer Murphy / Lecturer in Psychology, Royal Holloway University of London

Jenny is a lecturer in Psychology at Royal Holloway University of London. She completed her PhD at King's College London in 2020. Her doctoral achievements have been recognised by the award of an Elsevier Outstanding PhD Thesis prize, the EPS Frith Prize, the BPS Award and the Gottesman-Shields Award. Jenny is particularly interested in individual differences in interoception and how these differences link to mental and physical health.

By Freya Prentice / PhD Candidate at Great Ormond Street Institute of Child Health, UCL

Freya received her undergraduate degree in Experimental Psychology from the University of Oxford in 2018. She also completed an MSc in 2021 in Applied Paediatric Neuropsychology, with her thesis examing language in children with Rasmussen Syndrome. She is currently doing a PhD at UCL in Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience focusing on language plasticity in children with epilepsy.

Freya also worked as a research assistant focusing on sex and gender differences in interoception, and the implications for emotional ability and mental health.

(Source: theconversation.com; January 20, 2022; https://tinyurl.com/448afdwd)
Back to INF

Loading please wait...